G. Rizzolatti, Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.3, issue.2, pp.131-141, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0

G. Rizzolatti, Cortical mechanisms subserving object grasping and action recognition: a new view of the cortical motor functions, The New Cognitive Neuroscience (Gazzaniga, pp.539-552, 2000.

G. Rizzolatti, A unifying view of the basis of social cognition, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.8, issue.9, pp.396-403, 2004.

G. Rizzolatti, Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution, Experimental Brain Research, vol.111, issue.2, pp.246-252, 1996.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00227301

S. T. Grafton, Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 2. Observation compared with imagination, Experimental Brain Research, vol.112, pp.103-111, 1996.

J. Decety, Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy, Brain, vol.120, issue.10, pp.1763-1777, 1997.
DOI : 10.1093/brain/120.10.1763

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00655257

L. Fadiga, Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.73, pp.2608-2611, 1995.

V. Gallese, The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol.358, issue.1431, pp.159-18213, 2003.
DOI : 10.1098/rstb.2002.1234

D. Wolpert, A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction, The Neuroscience of Social Interaction, pp.305-322, 2003.
DOI : 10.1098/rstb.2002.1238

S. Blakemore and J. Decety, From the perception of action to the understanding of intention, Nature Neuroscience, vol.2, pp.561-567, 2001.

T. Metzinger and V. Gallese, The emergence of a shared action ontology: Building blocks for a theory, Consciousness and Cognition, vol.12, issue.4, pp.549-571, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00072-2

V. Gallese and A. Goldman, Mirror Neurons and the Simulation Theory of Mindreading, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.2, issue.12, pp.493-501, 1998.
DOI : 10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199874187.003.0003

M. Jeannerod, Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition, NeuroImage, vol.14, issue.1, pp.103-109, 2001.
DOI : 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832

P. Jacob and M. Jeannerod, Ways of Seeing, the Scope and Limits of Visual Cognition, p.14, 2003.

S. Blakemore, Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading?, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.8, issue.5, pp.216-221, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.012

S. Baron-cohen, Mindblindness: Essay on Autism and the Theory of Mind, Learning, Development & Conceptual Change The, 1995.

A. Leslie and L. Thaiss, Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism, Cognition, vol.43, issue.3, pp.225-251, 1992.
DOI : 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90013-8

U. 19-frith and C. D. Frith, Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing, The Neuroscience of Social Interaction, pp.45-75, 2003.
DOI : 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218

R. Saxe, Understanding Other Minds: Linking Developmental Psychology and Functional Neuroimaging, Annual Review of Psychology, vol.55, issue.1, pp.87-124, 2004.
DOI : 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142044

A. Gopnik and H. M. Wellman, The theory theory, Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, pp.257-294, 1994.
DOI : 10.1017/CBO9780511752902.011

R. Gordon, Folk Psychology as Simulation, Mind & Language, vol.1, issue.2, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

A. Goldman, Interpretation Psychologized, Folk Psychology (Davies, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

T. Allison, Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.4, issue.7, pp.267-278, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01501-1

A. Leslie and P. Polizzi, Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Two conjectures, Developmental Science, vol.1, issue.2, pp.247-253, 1998.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-7687.00038

A. L. Woodward, Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach, Cognition, vol.69, issue.1, pp.1-34, 1998.
DOI : 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4

J. Searle, Intentionality, an Essay in the Philosophy of Mind, 1983.

E. Pacherie, The content of intentions. Mind and Language 15, pp.400-432, 2000.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/ijn_00000258

H. P. Grice, Studies in the Way of, 1989.

G. Gergely, Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age, Cognition, vol.56, issue.2, pp.165-193, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-H

G. Csibra, Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ???pure reason??? in infancy, Cognition, vol.72, issue.3, pp.237-267, 1999.
DOI : 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00039-6

F. Heider and M. Simmel, An Experimental Study of Apparent Behavior, The American Journal of Psychology, vol.57, issue.2, pp.243-259, 1944.
DOI : 10.2307/1416950

F. Castelli, Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns, NeuroImage, vol.12, issue.3, pp.314-325, 2000.
DOI : 10.1006/nimg.2000.0612

D. S. Berry and K. Springer, Structure, Motion, and Preschoolers' Perceptions of Social Causality, Ecological Psychology, vol.62, issue.4, pp.273-283, 1993.
DOI : 10.1207/s15326969eco0302_2

V. 36-kuhlmeier, Attribution of Dispositional States by 12-Month-Olds, Psychological Science, vol.82, issue.5, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4

E. Spelke, Infants??? knowledge of object motion and human action, Causal Cognition, a Multidisciplinary Debate, pp.44-78, 1995.
DOI : 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524021.003.0003

R. Adolphs, Social cognition and the human brain, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.3, issue.12, pp.469-479, 1999.
DOI : 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01399-6

R. Adolphs, The human amygdala in social judgment, Nature, vol.393, issue.6684, pp.470-474, 1998.
DOI : 10.1038/30982

D. I. Perrett, Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex, Experimental Brain Research, vol.47, issue.3, pp.329-342, 1982.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00239352

D. I. Perrett, Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions, Journal of Experimental Biology, vol.146, pp.87-113, 1989.

C. 42-keysers and D. I. Perrett, The neural correlates of social perception: a Hebbian network perspective, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2004.

D. K. Hietanen and D. I. Perrett, Motion sensitive cells in the macaque superior temporal polysensory area. 1 Lack of response to the sight of the animal's own limb movement, Experimental Brain Research, vol.93, pp.117-128, 1993.

D. K. Hietanen and D. I. Perrett, Motion sensitive cells in the macaque superior temporal polysensory area: response discrimination between self-generated and externally generated pattern motion, Behavioural Brain Research, vol.76, issue.1-2, pp.155-167, 1996.
DOI : 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00193-X

G. Currie, Visual imagery as the simulation of vision. Mind and Language 10, pp.25-44, 1995.

M. Kumashio, Natural imitation induced by joint attention in Japanese monkeys, International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol.50, issue.1-2, pp.81-99, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00126-0

A. N. Meltzoff, Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children., Developmental Psychology, vol.31, issue.5, pp.838-850, 1995.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838

A. N. Meltzoff and J. Decety, What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience, The Neuroscience of Social Interaction, pp.109-130, 2003.
DOI : 10.1098/rstb.2002.1261

URL : http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1351349